Understanding the Effects of Early Investments in Children

07/13/2015
Featured in print Reporter
By Diane Whitmore Schanzenbach

A growing economics literature is seeking to understand the effects of early childhood influences on later life outcomes. While much recent work explores the effects of health measured at birth, my work and that of others demonstrates the importance of events in early life–but after birth–on long-term outcomes.

A recent review by Douglas Almond and Janet Currie concludes that child and family characteristics measured at school entry explain as much of the variation in adult outcomes as factors such as years of education that are more typically studied by economists.1 James Heckman argues that the rates of return to human capital investment in disadvantaged populations are highest in early life.2

In a series of studies, my coauthors and I have estimated the long-term impacts of interventions in early life. We find that there are promising interventions for children in school settings and through social safety net programs that impact outcomes measured in later adolescence and into adulthood.

Early Life Interventions and Adult Economic and Health Outcomes

The food stamp program is a central part of the U.S. safety net, and provides vouchers to participants that can be used to purchase food at grocery stores. Participation in the food stamp program increases the total resources available to a family, pushing out the budget constraint and raising consumption levels among participants. When the program was introduced in the 1960s, it was rolled out slowly, over a 13-year period, on a county-by-county basis. Hilary Hoynes, Almond, and I investigate the impact of this safety net transfer by exploiting this variation across geography and time of the introduction of the program.

One of our first studies found that babies who were in utero when food stamps were introduced in their county weighed more at birth.3 But the availability of food stamps at other points during childhood may also have had an impact. In our recent work, we use the Panel Study of Income Dynamics (PSID) to test whether children born prior to the introduction of food stamps in their county also benefited from the program.4 We start with the cohort of children that we initially observe in the 1968 PSID, follow them into adulthood, and observe their completed education, earnings, and detailed health outcomes such as general health status, height and weight, presence of chronic conditions, and work/activity limitations. We find that individuals with access to food stamps before age five had measurably better health in adulthood, exhibiting improved overall health and lower rates of obesity, diabetes, and high blood pressure.

For women, we also find that childhood access to the food stamp program increases economic self-sufficiency in adulthood. Those with access to food stamps as children were more likely to graduate from high school, earn more, and rely less on the social safety net as adults than those who did not. Interestingly, we find positive but diminishing impacts of food stamps by the child's age when the program was introduced in the child's county. These findings suggest that there are important long-term returns to family income from social supports during early life, and that income-support programs have benefits that have not previously been well quantified.

Expanding Access to Preschool

Another promising intervention in early life is access to high-quality educational environments. In his 2013 State of the Union address, President Obama proposed sweeping reform to preschool education in the United States. The $75 billion proposed "Preschool for All" initiative calls for dramatic increases in the number of four-year-olds in public preschool programs and in the quality of these programs nationwide.

The Preschool for All initiative shares many characteristics with the state universal preschool programs that have been offered in Georgia and Oklahoma since the 1990s. While high rates of return have been documented for preschool programs targeted to children of low socioeconomic status, less is known about the impacts of universal programs.5 Elizabeth Cascio and I draw together data from multiple sources to estimate the impacts of these "model" state programs on preschool enrollment and a broad set of short- and longer-run family and child outcomes.6

Using data from the Current Population Survey's October supplement on school enrollment, we find that the universal state programs have increased preschool enrollment rates of children from lower and higher socioeconomic status families alike. For children from lower-SES families, preschool enrollment increased by 20 percentage points, primarily driven by children who otherwise would not have attended preschool enrolling in public programs. For children from higher-SES families, however, the enrollment impact has been more muted at 11 to 14 percentage points. Among these higher-SES children, we measure substantial crowd out due to the universal program. In particular, we estimate that of every 10 higher-SES program enrollees, four or five otherwise would have been in private preschools.

We also document the impacts of universal preschool programs on money and time investments families make in their children. Using data from the Consumer Expenditure Survey, we find that higher-SES families–for whom private preschool enrollment falls significantly–reduce their spending on childcare. By contrast, lower-SES families–for whom there are larger impacts on the extensive margin–display larger declines in overall maternal time spent with children using data from the American Time Use Survey. This finding is counterbalanced by an increase in "quality" time spent reading, playing, talking, etc., between low-SES mothers and their children.

WhitmoreSummary
Figure 1

 

While the model state programs are still too young for us to estimate their long-term impacts, we are able to explore impacts on achievement as late as eighth grade. For lower-income children, we find positive impacts on math and reading scores as measured on the National Assessment of Education Progress (NAEP) in fourth grade. The impacts on this group diminish substantially by eighth grade, but sizable impacts on math scores remain. Conversely, although some children from higher-income families were more likely to have attended preschool and other families effectively received sizable income transfers from the program, their academic achievement does not appear to have improved in either grade.

We view these results as highly consistent with the broad patterns in the preschool literature, with highest economic returns coming from programs that are both high quality and highly targeted, and returns diminishing as the students' counter-factual experiences in the absence of a public preschool program increase.

Long-run Impacts of Early Elementary School Experiences

Another widely-discussed early-life intervention is improvement in the quality of schooling for students in the early elementary grades. Project STAR was a randomized experiment conducted in Tennessee in which over 11,000 students and their teachers were assigned to classrooms that varied in class size and other measures of quality. The experiment included one cohort of children that started kindergarten in the 1985–86 school year. By now, the students in the original cohort have become adults, and researchers have been able to use a variety of administrative databases to follow their progress to determine whether the intervention had meaningful lasting impacts.

Susan Dynarski, Joshua Hyman, and I measure the impact on educational attainment in adulthood of being randomly assigned in grades K-3 to a small class (with on average 15 students) instead of a larger class (with on average 22 students).7 Using a data match obtained from the National Student Clearinghouse, we find that assignment to a small class increases the probability of attending college by 2.7 percentage points.

The impacts are considerably higher among populations with traditionally low rates of postsecondary attainment. For example, the estimated impacts on college attendance are 5.8 percentage points among black students, and 4.4 percentage points among students who were eligible for a subsidized school lunch at the time of the original experiment. Among students attending schools with the highest concentration of poverty, small class assignment raised the rate of college attendance by 7.3 percentage points, and among students with the lowest projected probability of attending college, the impact is 11 percentage points. In addition, small classes in the early grades improve the likelihood of earning a college degree, and majoring in a more technical and high-earning field, such as science, technology, engineering, mathematics, business, or economics.

Using administrative tax return data, we are able to broaden the scope of adult outcomes studied in the Project STAR experiment. Raj Chetty, John Friedman, Nathaniel Hilger, Emmanuel Saez, Danny Yagan, and I analyzed the impacts of the experiment on outcomes ranging from earnings to retirement savings, home ownership, and marriage.8 While we study long-term impacts of random assignment to a variety of observable characteristics such as class size and teacher experience, we also estimate "classroom effects"–the combined effects of teachers, peers, and other class-level shocks–on later life outcomes. An analysis of variance revealed that kindergarten classroom assignment has significant impacts on earnings and other adult outcomes, leading us to ask whether the class effects on earnings were correlated with class effects on kindergarten test scores. To address this, we proxy for classroom quality with the average test scores of an individual’s classmates, measured at the end of kindergarten. Using this proxy measure, we find that class quality impacts immediate test scores, that the boost dissipates later in elementary school, and that it reappears strongly across a variety of adult outcomes including earnings. While the test-score impacts faded away in later grades, we were able to detect sustained impacts on non-cognitive skills, suggesting a possible mechanism for the long-term effects. The impacts of class quality are similar for students who entered the experiment later, suggesting that a better classroom environment from ages five through eight can have substantial long-term benefits.

In both Project STAR papers, we find that the actual long-run impacts were larger than what would have been predicted based on the short-run test score gains. This finding is consistent with a growing body of research on early-life interventions, and raises challenging evaluation problems. Policymakers often rely on short-term outcomes such as standardized test scores to gauge the effectiveness of educational interventions. But, as suggested in our research, if these measures systematically understate the long-run impacts of early childhood programs, over-reliance on short-run outcomes may lead to abandonment of some policies that would pass a long-term cost-benefit analysis.

Endnotes

1.

D. Almond and J. Currie, "Human Capital Development Before Age Five," NBER Working Paper 15827, March 2010, and O. Ashenfelter and D. Card, eds., Handbook of Labor Economics, 4B, Amsterdam, The Netherlands: North Holland, 2010, Chapter 15, pp. 1315-1486.
 

2.

J. J. Heckman and D. Masterov, "The Productivity Argument for Investing in Young Children," NBER Working Paper 13016, April 2007, and Review of Agricultural Economics, 29(3), 2007, pp. 446-93.

3.

D. Almond, H. W. Hoynes, and D. W. Schanzenbach, "Inside the War on Poverty: The Impact of Food Stamps on Birth Outcomes," NBER Working Paper 14306, September 2008, and Review of Economics and Statistics, 93(2), 2011, pp. 387-403.
 

4.

H. W. Hoynes, D. W. Schanzenbach, and D. Almond, "Long Run Impacts of Childhood Access to the Safety Net," NBER Working Paper 18535, November 2012.
 

5.

J. J. Heckman, S. H. Moon, R. Pinto, P. Savelyev, and A. Yavitz, "A New Cost-Benefit and Rate of Return Analysis for the Perry Preschool Program: A Summary," NBER Working Paper 16180, July 2010, and Journal of Public Economics, 94, 2010, pp. 114-28 (published as "The Rate of the Return to the HighScope Perry Preschool Program"); J. Ludwig and D. A. Phillips, "The Benefits and Costs of Head Start," NBER Working Paper 12973, March 2007, and Society for Research on Child Development, Social Policy Report, 21(3), 2007.
 

6.

E. U. Cascio and D. W. Schanzenbach, "The Impacts of Expanding Access to High-Quality Preschool Education," NBER Working Paper 19735, December 2013, and Brookings Papers on Economic Activity, 47(2), 2013, pp. 127-92.
 

7.

S. Dynarski, J. M. Hyman, and D. W. Schanzenbach, "Experimental Evidence on the Effect of Childhood Investments on Postsecondary Attainment and Degree Completion," NBER Working Paper 17533, October 2011, and Journal of Policy Analysis and Management, 32(4), 2013, pp. 692-717.
 

8.

R. Chetty, J. N. Friedman, N. Hilger, E. Saez, D. W. Schanzenbach, and D. Yagan, "How Does Your Kindergarten Classroom Affect Your Earnings? Evidence from Project STAR," NBER Working Paper 16381, September 2010, and Quarterly Journal of Economics, 126(4), 2011, pp. 1593-1660.